Saturday, May 28, 2016

Bandura’s Social Cognitive Theory: An Analysis part 9


https://cuevaseds103.files.wordpress.com/2015/10/bobo-experiment.jpg

            To prove that that all human behavior was learned through social imitation and copying, rather than inherited through genetic factors, Bandura performed the Bobo Doll Experiment in 1961(Shuttleworth, 2008).  This gave Bandura several ways to prove his hypothesis that watching violence causes aggressive behavior.  This conclusion, however, has been argued because the children, who are used as the subjects of the experiment, were said to be manipulated into responding aggressively according to what they have observed (“Criticisms”, n.d.).  If observing an aggressive behavior and being rewarded for it produces similar aggression, then would observing and watching a well-recognized diligent student produce the same attitude for everyone in the class? Perhaps, some students would not imbibe the same attitude.  In the real-world, adopting a behavior, whether good or bad, is subject to numerous factors.  Moreover, we cannot generalize how the majority, or even everyone, in the class would react, because each student has his/her own values, experiences, beliefs, and traditions.  People are different from each other.  Hence, if we say that people tend to have different reactions when it comes to what they observe, can we affirm that the Bobo doll experiment has been suggestive?  Yes, it is.  The children used in the experiment are aged between 3 to 6 years old (Bandura, Ross & Ross as cited by McLeod, 2014).  Since these children are very young and so they might not have any idea on how to play with the Bobo doll, they have to observe what others are doing with it.  It is not that the children were not using their thinking skills, but when the children are given with an unfamiliar thing without any instructions on how to use it, they would ask verbally or nonverbally: “What am I supposed to do with this thing?”.  The children, who were expose to a model with an aggressive behavior, in the Bobo doll experiment might have thought that the observed aggressive acts are the things that they were supposed to do in the experiment, and so they did. Although they did show an aggressive behavior, this does not mean that they would do the aggressive acts to other children.  Similarly, in teaching Edukasyon sa Pagpapakatao in the early grades, we tend to ask the pupils questions that are somewhat suggestive. For instance, when we ask the students similar questions like: “Nag-iingay ang iyong mga mga kamag-aral habang nagkaklaklase kayo, ano ang iyong gagawin?”.  The children would most probably think of an answer that would not make the teacher angry, so most likely, they would answer: “Sasawayin ko po ang aking mga kamag-aral”, even though in real life situation, they would not really do so.  Therefore, we cannot generalize that modeling and reinforcement does assure the replication of behavior at least with the children because, as mentioned earlier, there are a lot of things to consider before imbibing the behavior of the model, and people are just different from each other.  In addition, the manner of how we learn and behave changes as we grow up, because we tend to form deeper thoughts as our brains develop (generally speaking).  Bandura surprisingly agree with this statement.  In fact, he wrote in 1991:
  
            People possess self-reflective and self-reactive capabilities that enable them [to] exercise some control over their thoughts, feelings, motivations and actions.  In the exercise of self-directedness, people adopt certain standards of behavior that serve as guides and motivators and regulate their actions anticipatorily through self-reactive influence.  Human functioning is, therefore, regulated by an interplay of self-generated and external sources of influence (Ananda, 2006, para. 2).